https://nova.newcastle.edu.au/vital/access/ /manager/Index ${session.getAttribute("locale")} 5 An embedded randomised controlled trial of a Teaser Campaign to optimise recruitment in primary care https://nova.newcastle.edu.au/vital/access/ /manager/Repository/uon:30546 Wed 11 Apr 2018 12:58:57 AEST ]]> Feeling reassured after a consultation does not reduce disability or healthcare use in people with acute low back pain: a mediation analysis of a randomised trial https://nova.newcastle.edu.au/vital/access/ /manager/Repository/uon:53402 Thu 23 Nov 2023 13:43:36 AEDT ]]> Producing Clinically Meaningful Reductions in Disability: A Causal Mediation Analysis of a Patient Education Intervention https://nova.newcastle.edu.au/vital/access/ /manager/Repository/uon:44793 Mon 24 Oct 2022 09:17:55 AEDT ]]> Feasibility of an Audit and Feedback Intervention to Facilitate Journal Policy Change Towards Greater Promotion of Transparency and Openness in Sports Science Research https://nova.newcastle.edu.au/vital/access/ /manager/Repository/uon:51932 Fri 22 Sep 2023 11:06:57 AEST ]]> The reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals https://nova.newcastle.edu.au/vital/access/ /manager/Repository/uon:36214 .70 indicates adequate reliability). The ICCs(2, 1) ranged from -.31 to.71. Reliability varied according to the outcome measure and threat word category. Sensory words had a lower mean ICC (.08) than either affective words (.32) or general threat words (.29). A longer exposure time was associated with higher test-retest reliability. All of the outcome measures, except second-run dwell time, demonstrated low measurement error (<6%). Most of the outcome measures reported high internal consistency (α >.93). Recommendations are discussed for improving the reliability of eyetracking tasks in future research.]]> Fri 06 Mar 2020 12:36:45 AEDT ]]>